Recognition, assessment and scoring of pain in dogs and cats (Proceedings)

Article

It is now a settled matter that the adaptive capacities of animals, coupled with the innate biases of human observers, seriously impairs our ability to "know" which of our patients are in pain, how much they are in pain, and sometimes, even where they are in pain. Historically the absence of behaviors easily associated with pain (crying, whimpering, etc.) has been equated with the absence of pain.

It is now a settled matter that the adaptive capacities of animals, coupled with the innate biases of human observers, seriously impairs our ability to "know" which of our patients are in pain, how much they are in pain, and sometimes, even where they are in pain. Historically the absence of behaviors easily associated with pain (crying, whimpering, etc.) has been equated with the absence of pain. In fact, animals can lay quietly in pain, or conversely act painful until the approach of a person whereupon the painful behavior is replaced by a happy, tail-wagging greeting. Therefore it is first incumbent for veterinary clinicians to operate under the assumption that some procedures and conditions are inherently painful, without the patient having to "prove" they are in pain. Any surgical procedure, trauma, and many medical conditions such as gastroenteritis, pancreatitis, cystitis, inflammatory bowel disease, and certain neoplasms are all examples of where it is incumbent upon the clinician to include pain management in their treatment plan.

Pain is complex, experiential, individual, and multifactoral in its expression. Nevertheless, scoring pain is increasingly recognized as the "4th vital sign" in animals after temperature, pulse, and respiration (in humans it is the 5th, after blood pressure). Indeed the AAHA certification guidelines now require this assessment on every patient (MA23, PM1).

Overt signs of pain vary by species, and even within species some breeds and individuals have enhanced local and descending inhibitory mechanisms when compared others. Also, realize that pet owners may be better at "reading" their pet than a stranger. Listed below, are just some of the "new onset pain behaviors" shown by animals in acute severe pain. (adapted from Karol Matthews excellent chapter in the Veterinary Clinics of North America Small Animal Management of Pain, July 2000 Vol 30 issue entitled: Pain Assessment and General Approach to Management). However, the observer must be very astute if they are to pick up signs of moderate or mild pain, and are exhibited not by the onset of new behaviors, but rather absence of usual behaviors.

DOGS: abnormal sitting or lying posture, restlessness, thrashing, splinting of abdomen, "prayer position," whining, groaning or otherwise vocalizing, limping, unwilling to get up, unwilling to lie down, lack of appetite, trembling, increased respiratory rate, expiratory gruntbulging eyes, dilated pupils, aggression, resents being touched, dull behavior, won't wag tail, licking or biting at affected area, lack of grooming

CATS: can be seen with many of the signs listed above for dogs, and cats are more likely to be in a crouched position or grumpy when they are in pain. However, some cats actually purr when they are distressed or in pain! Cats in severe postoperative pain may become aggressive, tearing at the bandage, frantic and vocalizing. Also, cats are more likely to exhibit withdrawn behavior – crouching in the back of the cage, unwilling to use the litter box, etc., than dogs.

An investigational tool in human medicine is evaluation of facial expressions, on the premise that certain muscle movements universally occur in our species when pain is present; this has been called "the primal face of pain." Computer software programs are being developed to detect these expressions that are then translated into a automated score. Similar primal facial expressions of pain are thought to exist in animals, e.g. furrowed brow, squinted eyes, ears turned back or away from the forward position, and even without a computer program these subtle changes can provide additional information to the veterinary observer.

Scoring pain in non-verbal patients is a special challenge, examples of which includes not only animals but neonates infant children and incapacitated (physically or mentally) adult humans. Measuring objective physiologic parameters has proven to be unreliable as indicators of pain largely because of the influence of other non-pain influences e.g. stress, distress, anxiety, and normal biologic variation. Therefore we are left with subjective evaluations and such scoring systems should meet the following criteria:

  • Interobserver variability and observer bias is minimized

  • Distinguish between varying levels of pain intensity

  • The degree of "importance" of pain to the subject is detected.

Several terms that refer to qualities of a pain rating tool include: validity (the ability of the scale to effectively measure what it is supposed to measure), responsiveness (whether the tool can detect a change in pain, particularly one that is meaningful to the subject), and reliability (whether two observers will give similar ratings using the tool). A pain scale should also ideally be multidimensional, in that several aspects of the pain intensity or pain related disability are rated. Pain is felt to have multiple dimensions, and in human chronic pain rating tools, subjects may be asked a number of different questions about their pain - to rate the intensity, to describe how much it interferes with work or with family relationships, and to indicate how unpleasant the pain is. The importance of, or degree of unpleasantness of pain, or of any symptom may be related to the impact on survival that a given symptom has.

Despite the challenges, advances have been made in the use of behavioral evaluations to assign acute (especially post-operative) pain scores, falling under the following general categories:

  • SDS (Simple Descriptive Scale), where the observer picks value on a 0-4 or 0-10 scale (no pain to mild pain to moderate pain to severe pain to worst possible pain) that most accurately describes the patient.

  • VAS (Visual Analogue Scale), where a point is picked along a graduated 0-100mm "ruler", with 0 being no pain and 100 being the worst possible pain; classically employed, it can only be used by a patient that can self-report (i.e. is verbal, not animals, neonates, stroke victims, etc.), although sometimes an observer is used to pick the spot as a proxy, as would be necessary in veterinary medicine.

  • DIVAS, an expansion of the VAS which includes dynamic (the animal is aspected to move) and interactive (the wound is palpated) observations

  • NRS (Numerical Rating Scale), where values are assigned to several designated clinical criteria, the sum of which gives the pain score.

Once verbal descriptions of behavior become part of pain scales, then care must be taken to ensure that all potential users of the scale assign equal meaning and importance to the words or terms in the description. Arbitrary word meanings can increase the subjectivity of the measurement. With both VAS and NRS the observer's skill of evaluation is crucial to accurate assessment. Although an observation is recorded rapidly, VAS, NRS and SDS are considered to have poor reliability when multiple, less skilled observers are using them; advantages and disadvantages are commonly discussed. Many observers will never have seen an animal experiencing the "worst possible pain", and so the VAS scale is subject to bias. In addition, the values that are generated from these scales are non-numerical, thus it cannot necessarily be assumed that a subject whose pain was rated at 8 was twice a painful as one whose pain was rated at 4.

The advantages of the SDS and the VAS systems are inherently found in their simplicity and ease of use. However, they also historically result in the most variability, especially between observers. However, both inter- and intra-observer variability is diminished by adding in dynamic and interactive observation, that is, without and then with patient interaction, asking the patient to move, and palpation of the surgical site.

A pain scale that would take into account the various dimensions of pain would be theoretically more useful in indicating how much the pain "meant" to the animal, but VAS, NRS and SDS scales are said to be unidimensional. An alternative, multidimensional type of scale is the composite measurement scale (CMS), constructed such that it takes into account such dimensions as the temporal patterns, location, interference with basic function, or of enjoyment of life. These have been widely developed for human patients although these require the ability to answer questions on the impact pain is having on them. In veterinary medicine we would instead use a basic but essential understanding of pain rating tools and animal behavior, and combining those with actual "in the trenches" experience with observations. Such scales would ideally "ask" the subject to evaluate how much their pain meant by observing their willingness to do context specific things,. In theory, the CMS approach of looking at several indicators of well-being (e.g., posture, body weight or food intake, motor impairment, evoked pain responses and social interaction, for example) might also hedge against the problem of a single measure being confounded by unexpected factors.

The CMS considered most validated and in most widespread use in veterinary medicine is the Glasgow Composite Measure Pain Scale (CMPS). It identified and defined over 100 descriptive terms used by veterinarians to describe the behavior of dogs experiencing discomfort, and placed them into categories. These numerous descriptors were designed to minimize bias and interpretation by observer; it has been found to be most accurate for acute musculo-skeletal (trauma) and post-operative pain, rather than painful medical conditions. It has been published as a long (and rather time-consuming) form, and more recently as a short form which speeds patient evaluation considerably. Each of several categories of descriptors are then scored, and the combined value gives the final score. The maximum score is 24, and the closer to this maximum the more painful the patient.

There are several advantages that are common to the various scoring systems such as the Glasgow CMPS. One is that they force the observer to evaluate the patient in ways they may not ordinarily undertake. Another is that they recognize that animals will behave differently when they have human

Whatever system is used, it should include the following features 1) an observation of the patient without interacting, 2) an observation of the patient while interacting, and 3) palpation of the painful site. The author uses a hybrid DIVAS/SDS system for its simplicity, however inclusive of the 3 features above, and placing overall assessment of pain on a 0-10 scale.

Summary

Scoring pain in animals, as in non-verbal humans, is a special challenge. Nevertheless it provides opportunities to place pain recognition and pain control in top of mind awareness, and thus has the promise of improving patient outcomes. Post-surgical pain scoring systems should be simple yet accounting account for the multi-dimensional experience and individual variability of pain expression. To minimize intra- and inter-observer variability a pain scoring system should evaluate new behaviors related to pain as well as normal behaviors not being exhibited, through the casual observation followed by interaction with the patient, asking it to move, and palpation of the surgical site. The Glasgow Composite Measure Pain Scale Score (Short Form) is such a validated system in veterinary medicine.

References:

Hansen BD. Assessment of pain in dogs: veterinary clinical studies. ILAR J. 2003; 44(3):197-205

AAHA Accreditation Guidelines, AAHA Press 2006

Schiavenato M, Byers JF, Scovanner P, McMahon JM, Xia Y, Lu N, He H. Neonatal pain facial expression: evaluating the primal face of pain. Pain. 2008 Aug 31;138(2):460-71. Epub 2008 Aug 9

Gessler P, Cignacco E. Measures for the assessment of pain in neonates as well as a comparison between the Bernese Pain Scale for Neonates (BPSN) with the Premature Infant Pain Profile (PIPP) Klin Padiatr. 2004 Jan-Feb;216(1):16-20.

Suraseranivongse S, Kaosaard R et al. A comparison of postoperative pain scales in neonates. Br J Anaesth. 2006 Oct;97(4):540-4. Epub 2006 Aug 1

Crellin D, Sullivan TP, et al. Analysis of the validation of existing behavioral pain and distress scales for use in the procedural setting. Paediatr Anaesth. 2007 Aug;17(8):720-33.

Aubin M, Giguère A, et al. The systematic evaluation of instruments designed to assess pain in persons with limited ability to communicate. Pain Res Manag. 2007 Autumn;12(3):195-203

Cambridge AJ, Tobias KM et al, Subjective and objective measurements of postoperative pain in cats, J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2000 Sep 1;217(5):685-90

Holton LL, et al, Relationship between physiological factors and clinical pain in dogs scored using a numerical Rating scale J Small Anim Pract 1998; 39(10):469-474

Smith JD, Allen SW, Quandt JE, Changes in cortisol concentration in response to stress and postoperative pain in client-owned cats and correlation with objective clinical variables. Am J Vet Res 1999; 60:432-436

Hudson JT, Slater MR, et al. Assessing repeatability and validity of a visual analogue scale questionnaire for use in assessing pain and lameness in dogs. Am J Vet Res. 2004 Dec;65(12):1634-43.

Hellyer PW and Gaynor JS: How I treat: Acute Postsurgical pain in dogs and cats. Comp Contin Educ 20:140-153, 1998

Holton LL, Scott EM, et al, Comparison of three methods used for assessment of pain in dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 1998 Jan 1;212(1):61-6

Pascoe PJ. Pain Management in the Canine Patient, Proceedings 9th Annual Canine Pain Management Medicine Symposium, 2006

Holton L, Reid J. et al: The development of a behavioral based pain scale to measure acute pain in dogs. Vet Rec 148:525-531, 2001

Reid, J; Scott, M; et al. Development of a short form of the Glasgow Composite Measure Pain Scale (CMPS) as a measure of acute pain in the dog. Proceedings of the Association of Veterinary Anaesthetists, Spring Meeting, 2005; 59

Recent Videos
Related Content
© 2024 MJH Life Sciences

All rights reserved.